Skip to main content
Kyoto University » Primate Research Institute

Search form

CICASP | Center for International Collaboration and Advanced Studies in Primatology

  • About CICASP
  • Program
    • Course Details
    • Application
    • FAQ
    • Other Programs
    • Funding
    • Testimonials
    Stay
  • Departments

    Departments and Staff

    • Center for the Evolutionary Origins of Human Behavior (EHUB)
    • CICASP
    • Kyoto University Museum
    • The Center for Ecological Research
    • Center for Human Evolution Modeling Research
    • Wildlife Research Center
    Stay
  • CICASP Staff
    • CICASP
    • Administration
    • Students
    • Alumni
    Stay
  • Education

    View: Education

    • Fundamental Lectures
    • Inuyama Nerds
    • Other Educational Events
    • Sci Comm Workshops
    Stay
  • News & Media
    • Announcements
    • Articles
    • Events
    • Interviews
    • Podcasts
    • Publications
    Stay
  • Campus Life
    • Around Inuyama
    • Around Nagoya
    • Festivals
    • On Campus
    Stay
  • Contact
HomeNewsArticles
  • December 21st 2022

    The PrimateCast 76: Dr. Elaine Guevara on Primate Eponyms

    Listen
  • December 9th 2022

    The PrimateCast Origins (75): Professor Mewa Singh on his half-century journey into primatology and wildlife biology

    Listen
  • November 17th 2022

    The PrimateCast (74): Dr. Briana Pobiner on what makes us human, paleontological time machines and bigging up science education

    Listen
  • November 1st 2022

    The PrimateCast Origins (73): Dr. John Mitani on his life among the apes

    Listen
  • October 19th 2022

    The PrimateCast 72: Dr. Charles (Chuck) Snowdon on what music means to us, and monkeys!

    Listen
  • October 12th 2022

    The PrimateCast 71: Dr. Pamela Asquith on language, anthropomorphism, and metaphor in science, and translating Kinji Imanishi and the flow of Japanese primatology

    Listen
  • August 21st 2022

    The PrimateCast #70: Dr. Karen Strier on weaving between theory and practice in behavioral ecology and conservation

    Listen
  • July 27th 2022
    Takeshi Furuichi with local kids at Wamba Village in the DRC

    The PrimateCast #69: Dr. Takeshi Furuichi on bonobos, Wamba Village in the DRC, and building theories of human behavioral evolution

    Listen
  • July 1st 2022
    Dr. Elisabetta Visalberghi on the PrimateCast

    The PrimateCast #68: Dr. Elisabetta Visalberghi on Being a Primate, Becoming a Primatologist

    Listen
  • June 17th 2022
    Susumu Tomiya descends into Natural Trap Cave, Wyoming

    The PrimateCast #67: Dr. Susumu Tomiya on paleontology, the past, present and future of biodiversity, beardogs, and doing and communicating science

    Listen
  • May 8th 2022

    The PrimateCast #66: Dr. Robin Dunbar on how the social brain evolved

    Listen
  • April 22nd 2022

    The PrimateCast #65: Dr. Ikuma Adachi on Comparative Cognition and Managing a Chimpanzee Research Program

    Listen

The PrimateCast

The PrimateCast - Podcast
Catch interviews from the world of primatology, wildlife science and beyond with The PrimateCast. Available here or on iTunes. Subscribe to our rss feed, add us on iTunes and follow us on social media at Facebook and Twitter @ThePrimateCast. View all Podcasts
Subscribe to The PrimateCast
 
 
iTunes Channel
 

Topics

  • Announcements (22)
  • Articles (3)
  • Events (23)
  • Interviews (1)
  • Podcasts (77)
  • Publications (28)

Recent News

International Primatology Lecture 18: Prof. Frans de Waal
January 12th 2023
Read More >
International Primatology Lecture 17: Dr. Janette Wallis
November 25th 2022
Read More >
International Primatology Lecture 16 | Prof. James Anderson
October 20th 2022
Read More >
View all News ›

Monthly Archive

  • January 2023 (1)
  • December 2022 (2)
  • November 2022 (3)
  • October 2022 (3)
  • August 2022 (3)
  • July 2022 (3)

Follow Us

  • CICASP on Facebook
  • Follow CICASP on Twitter
  • CICASP on LinkedIn
  • CICASP on YouTube
  • CICASP RSS

Foundational studies in primatology & wildlife science: 2020 edition

October 12th 2020
Articles

Scientific progress is usually incremental—the fruits of all the hard work that we do as scientists everyday and every night come in little niblets. But once in a long while (and at least once in our lifetime, we all hope), a major breakthrough happens that fundamentally changes how we see the world, and how we think about the world. How do those advances come about?

To get us thinking, we asked the participants of our CICASP Seminar in Science Communication to nominate publications that they considered to be foundational in their own fields of research. Their selections include the true classics as well as ‘future classics’ for students of primatology and wildlife science. Do you see a common thread through these works?

 

1. Rollin, B.E. (1990) Animal welfare, animal rights, and agriculture, Journal of Animal Science 68:3456–3461.

“From the 1970s, a revolutionary change happened in the study of animal welfare and the laws made to protect them. More specifically, the paper discusses how we interact with animals and how an animal’s nature should be taken into consideration to uphold its psychological well-being. The author evaluated the legal and moral standings of animals in research, and how much more work was needed in agriculture. More importantly, he discussed how to interact, persuade and discuss animal welfare with various types of people and institutions that might have been apprehensive in the past or were still unable to change.”

—Josue Alejandro (PRI doctoral student in Social Systems Evolution)

 

2. Trut, L.N. (1999) Early canid domestication: the farm-fox experiment, American Scientist 87:160–169.

“This paper goes over the history of the Russian Farm Fox Experiment. This study has been hugely influential in the study of domestication, and is still referenced today as an example of domestication syndrome, a suite of traits that are common in domesticated species but not their wild counterparts.

In the 1950s, Dmitry K. Belyaev started a selective breeding program meant to domesticate silver foxes. The goal was to see if selecting only for tamer behavior would also select for other traits associated with domestication. Belyaev categorized foxes into 3 categories based on their reaction to experimenters, eventually adding a 4th category of exceptionally friendly foxes. Within a few generations, the proportion of foxes in the breeding program with traits associated with domestication like white coloring, floppy ears and short curly tails became much higher than in other silver fox populations.”

—Scott H. Jenkins (WRC master’s student)

 

3. Jones, C.G., J.H. Lawton, M. Shachak (1994) Organisms as ecosystem engineers, Oikos 69:373–386.

“This paper introduced the concept of ecosystem engineers. The authors formed the theoretical frameworks for studying ecosystem engineering by classifying ecosystem engineers, comparing it with the concept of keystone species. This study provides a distinct perspective and theoretical framework for an ecological process that has long been noticed, and inspired many studies in the following years.”

—Tianmeng He (PRI doctoral student in Ecology & Conservation)

 

4. Miller, G.A. (1956) The magical number 7 plus or minus 2: Some limits on our capacity for processing information, Psychological Review 63:81–97.

“In his article, Miller discussed a coincidence between the limits of one-dimensional absolute judgment and the limits of short-term memory. In a one-dimensional absolute-judgment task, a person was presented with a number of stimuli that varied on one dimension, and their response to each stimulus was measured. He then studied memory spans by determining the longest list of items that a person could repeat back in the correct order on 50% of trials immediately after presentation. He observed that the memory span of young adults was approximately seven items.

Though influential in several ways, for about 40 years it was oddly followed by rather little research on the numerical limit of capacity in working memory, or on the relationship among the three potentially related phenomena that Miller described.”

 —Sanjana Kadam (PRI master’s student in Cognition & Learning)

 

5. Darwin, C. (1872) The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals (John Murray, London).

“In this book, Darwin introduced the idea that each emotion is discrete. Most of his work on expression of emotions focused on the face and partially on voice and body posture. He proposed that emotional expressions are universal. He also introduced the methodology of studying emotions using facial expressions.”

—Srishti Tripathi (PRI doctoral student in Cognition & Learning)

 

6. Meyerhof, W., C. Batram, C. Kuhn, A. Brockhoff, E. Chudoba, B. Bufe, G. Appendino, M. Behrens. (2010) The Molecular Receptive Ranges of Human TAS2R Bitter Taste Receptors, Chemical Senses 35:150–170.

“Tasting bitterness is an innate rejection response to toxic food. Depending on the species, vertebrate genomes contain between 3 and 50 bitter-taste receptors called TAS2Rs. That means that the bitter taste receptors vary among species, and that each species can perceive many substances through bitterness. The authors systematically investigated the molecular receptive ranges of hTAS2Rs, and reported the ligands for most human TAS2R bitter taste receptors. By comparing the ways in which various vertebrates perceive bitterness, this study provides important clues to the pathways for the evolution of dietary behaviors.”

 —Rena Numabe (PRI master’s student in Molecular Biology)

 

7. Jones, K.E., N.G. Patel, M.A. Levy, A. Storeygard, D. Balk, J.L. Gittleman, P. Daszak. (2008) Global trends in emerging infectious diseases, Nature 451:990–993.

“The authors showed that emerging infectious diseases (EIDs) were dominated by zoonosis (60%), of which 72% originated from wildlife, and that EID “events” - new pathogen species or strain starting to infect humans - were significantly increasing over time. They also found that zoonotic and vector-borne EIDs were more concentrated in lower-latitude developing countries, whereas antimicrobial drug resistant bacteria was mostly originating from densely populated developed areas of higher latitudes. 

This paper points to the critical needs for health monitoring, better allocation of financial and research resources, conservation of areas rich in wildlife diversity by reducing anthropogenic activity, and better understanding of the factors that increase contact between wildlife and humans and their impact on disease emergence. It’s easy to read, important to wildlife science, and presents a strong argument for the trends.”

 —Kenneth Keuk (PRI doctoral student in Social Systems Evolution)

 

8. Eriksson, P.S., E. Perfilieva, T. Björk-Eriksson, A.-M. Alborn, C. Nordborg, D.A. Peterson, F. H. Gage. (1998) Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus, Nature Medicine 4:1313–1317.

“This study is about adult neurogenesis in the human brain. Driven by the long-standing misconception that adult neurogenesis in primates is impossible, this study shows that neuronal differentiation occurs in the subventricular zone (SVZ), hilus, and granule cell layer (GCL) of the hippocampus. Researchers used postmortem brain tissue from cancer patients who received injections of BrdU, which is a marker of dividing cells.

The value of this study to science is twofold: first, it is the first direct evidence that the adult human hippocampus can generate new neurons many decades into its life; second, it also presents various evolutionary implications associated with the phenomenon, such as behavioral flexibility and adaptability, that are still subjects of debate today.”

—Yoko Yamashita (PRI intern student in Phylogeny & Systematics)

 

9. Trivers, R.L. (1974) Parent-offspring conflict, American Zoologist 14:249–264.

“The author presents a theoretical framework for the study of parent-offspring conflicts. Before this paper, the presumption was that parents and offspring aimed for the same resource or energy allocation—i.e., parents would provide the amount of food that their offspring wanted. Trivers proposed that, instead, this was a conflict with each offspring demanding more and the parents trying to distribute the resources equally amongst all offspring.

This paper brought about a swift shift in how researchers viewed parent-offspring relation and evolution. It inspired behavioural ecologists to think about the evolutionary consequences of this conflict and in particular to mix game-theoretic modelling and behavioural experimentation.”

—Raquel Costa (PRI doctoral student in Language & Intelligence)

Happy reading!

  • Kyoto University

Navigation

  • About CICASP
  • Program
  • People
  • News & Media
  • Campus Life
  • Contact

Follow Us

  • CICASP on Facebook
  • Follow CICASP on Twitter
  • CICASP on LinkedIn
  • CICASP on YouTube
  • CICASP RSS
^Top

Contact Information

CICASP, 41-2, Kanrin,
Inuyama, Aichi,
484-8506 Japan
Phone: +81 (0)568-63-0284
Fax: +81 (0)568-61-1050
Email: cicasp [at] mail2 [dot] adm [dot] kyoto-u [dot] ac [dot] jp

© 2023 Center for International Collaboration and Advanced Studies in Primatology, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University